2020. 1. 25. 03:00ㆍ카테고리 없음
- Art Perception And Appreciation By Ma Aurora Ortiz Pdf 2017
- Art Perception And Appreciation By Ma Aurora Ortiz Pdf Download
- Art Perception And Appreciation By Ma Aurora Ortiz Pdf Free
Little about the neuropsychology of art perception and evaluation is known. Most neuropsychological approaches to art have focused on art production and have been anecdotal and qualitative.
Dynamic and Secure Straightforward PDF documents in WordPress Embed mobile-friendly PDFs easily in WordPress - and prevent your viewers downloading or printing your original files. Art Gallery Canvas Art Prints Metal Art Matching Sets Wood Wall Art Wall Sculptures Acrylic Wall Art.
The field is in desperate need of quantitative methods if it is to advance. Here, we combine a quantitative approach to the assessment of art with modern voxel-lesion-symptom-mapping methods to determine brain–behavior relationships in art perception. We hypothesized that perception of different attributes of art are likely to be disrupted by damage to different regions of the brain.
Twenty participants with right hemisphere damage were given the Assessment of Art Attributes, which is designed to quantify judgments of descriptive attributes of visual art. Each participant rated 24 paintings on 6 conceptual attributes (depictive accuracy, abstractness, emotion, symbolism, realism, and animacy) and 6 perceptual attributes (depth, color temperature, color saturation, balance, stroke, and simplicity) and their interest in and preference for these paintings. Deviation scores were obtained for each brain-damaged participant for each attribute based on correlations with group average ratings from 30 age-matched healthy participants.
Right hemisphere damage affected participants’ judgments of abstractness, accuracy, and stroke quality. Damage to areas within different parts of the frontal parietal and lateral temporal cortices produced deviation in judgments in four of six conceptual attributes (abstractness, symbolism, realism, and animacy). Of the formal attributes, only depth was affected by inferior prefrontal damage. No areas of brain damage were associated with deviations in interestingness or preference judgments. The perception of conceptual and formal attributes in artwork may in part dissociate from each other and from evaluative judgments. More generally, this approach demonstrates the feasibility of quantitative approaches to the neuropsychology of art. Assessment of art attributes scale We used the AAA battery to obtain a quantitative measure of individuals’ abilities to judge perceptual and conceptual qualities of visual art.
Art Perception And Appreciation By Ma Aurora Ortiz Pdf 2017
The AAA measures one’s ability to perceive 12 different attributes (6 formal and 6 conceptual) of visual art (Chatterjee et al., ). Participants rate 24 images of paintings from the Western art historical canon (Table ) on each of the 12 scales on a 5-point Likert scale. Before beginning the AAA battery, participants look at each image to orient themselves to the range of styles of paintings they would be rating. In order to define the dimensions of each attribute scale, participants first see a training slide and two example images that illustrate the extremes of the scale.
Participants are allowed to ask clarification questions before proceeding. Participants then rate each of the images of paintings on a 5-point Likert scale.
Images are presented in random order and no time limit is imposed. After all images have been rated on the 12 formal and conceptual qualities, participants then evaluate the paintings. They rate each painting for preference and for interest on a 5-point Likert scale. Lesion data Every patient’s lesion was drawn on a standard brain template (“Colin 27” from the MNI) by one of two senior neurologists. Using MRICron, lesions are defined with respect to anatomically defined structures (e.g., inferior frontal gyrus, angular gyrus, etc.) as well as Brodmann areas using Automated Anatomical Labeling and Brodmann Areas maps available in the MRICro software package (Rorden and Karnath, ). VLSM correlations were assessed by regressing behavioral scores on lesion status scores across subjects independently for each voxel. Only voxels that included at least two participants with brain damage analyzed using a false discovery rate of 0.01 (Figure ).
Analysis and results The group results of the screening tasks are shown in Table. In general patients did well on these tasks. Data from age and art-experience matched control participants were used to develop a baseline measure of esthetic perception and evaluation. For each scale, the 24 paintings were assigned a unique rank order based on their average rating by control participants. Then, individual control participants’ scores were correlated with the rank order for each scale using Spearman’s Rho, as a measure of non-parametric correlation. If an individual’s Rho statistic fell two SD below the average Rho on a particular scale, their ratings were not used in establishing the rank order of paintings for that attribute. See Chatterjee et al.
for details of these procedures. The results of this analysis demonstrate the reliability of the AAA. Controls tended to have high average Spearman’s Rho and low SE for each of the 12 attribute scales and interest and preference ratings (Table ).
Control mean rho Control SE Mean rho deviation (Control – patient) Deviation SE PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTES Balance 0.50 0.04 −0.15 0.05 Color saturation 0.57 0.04 −0.04 0.05 Color temperature 0.59 0.04 −0.22 0.08 Depth 0.54 0.03 −0.09 0.05 Simplicity 0.71 0.03 −0.10 0.04 Stroke. 0.66 0.05 −0.30 0.09 CONCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTES Abstractness. 0.81 0.02 −0.25 0.07 Animacy 0.71 0.03 −0.11 0.05 Emotion 0.61 0.03 −0.05 0.04 Objective accuracy.
0.76 0.02 −0.33 0.10 Realism 0.77 0.02 −0.11 0.04 Symbolism 0.69 0.02 −0.22 0.08 Interest 0.51 0.04 −0.16 0.06 Preference 0.47 0.04 −0.02 0.05. denotes attributes in which deviation of patients is significantly different at p. Discussion Our study examined how brain damage affects the perception and evaluation of art. We were motivated to demonstrate that quantitative approaches in the neuropsychology of art are feasible.
Our study is only a first step in this direction. In what follows, we shall mention the advantages of this approach and outline our results. We then discuss some limits of the study, and how the field might move forward. The results demonstrate that the neuropsychology of art can be investigated in a systematic and quantitative manner. We have shown previously that art production can be approached quantitatively (Smith et al., ).
Art Perception And Appreciation By Ma Aurora Ortiz Pdf Download
Now, we extend this approach to art perception. Quantitative approaches have the advantage of allowing formal tests of hypotheses and replication.
These advantages do not denigrate the qualitative insights one might derive from careful observation and theoretical analyses of art. However, it is hard to see how the neuropsychology of art could mature as a science without quantification (Chatterjee, ). Our study incorporates quantification in two ways. First, is the use of the AAA. This assessment allows quantification of specific attributes of any artwork (Chatterjee et al., ). There is nothing about the assessment that restricts its use to neuropsychology. The assessment could just as easily be used for other purposes, such as to compare the work of different artists or to assess the nature of change in any given artists’ style over time.
Art Perception And Appreciation By Ma Aurora Ortiz Pdf Free
Second, is our use of VLSM techniques (Bates et al., ). This method represents a general advance in lesion analyses and is being applied to the perception of art for the first time.
Our basic findings are that damage to the right hemisphere can affect the perception of selective aspects of art (see Table ). This cohort of patients as a group had impaired performance when judging the content-conceptual attributes of abstractness and depictive accuracy and the formal-perceptual attribute of stroke quality. We also found that damage to lateral frontal–parietal–temporal cortices was associated with deviations in the judgment of 4/6 content-conceptual art attributes: abstractness, symbolism, realism, and animacy. Of the formal-perceptual attributes, only depth was correlated with damage to the inferior prefrontal cortex. The fact that the patients as a group were impaired in judging depictive accuracy and stroke quality and these attributes did not show specific brain–behavior correlations, suggests the following hypothesis.
Judging the attributes of depictive accuracy and stroke quality maybe especially vulnerable to right brain damage in different locations and these attributes may instantiated non-linearly in the brain. No brain area was associated with deviations in judgments in evaluating preference or interestingness of these artworks.
Elsewhere, we have argued that any esthetic experience is built upon at least three components (Chatterjee, ). These components are the experiences of the sensory qualities, the associated sets of meanings, and the emotional responses evoked by the esthetic object. Broadly, one might regard the formal-perceptual attributes of the AAA as probing the sensory experience, the content-conceptual attributes as probing the meaning, and the evaluative questions as probing the emotional response to these paintings. From our data, we would tentatively propose that these three components of visual esthetic experiences segregate broadly in the organization of the brain. Most of our participants had damage in the distribution of the right middle cerebral artery.
This distribution of brain damage involving lateral frontal, parietal, and temporal cortices was more likely to affect judgments of conceptual attributes. We would predict that damage in the posterior cerebral artery distribution affecting ventral occipital and temporal cortices might be more likely to affect perceptual attributes. Furthermore, given the extensive data implicating the ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex in assigning subjective reward values (Kable and Glimcher, ), we would predict that damage to ventro-medial prefrontal cortices would be more likely to affect people’s evaluation of paintings. We should be clear about the limits of this study. A general limit is that we have relatively little experimental control over ways that broad cultural and sociological factors might contribute to how people apprehend art. One would expect that cultural factors would be more likely to produce variance in judgments of content-conceptual attributes than formal-perceptual attributes (Chatterjee, ). Yet, w note that the content-conceptual attributes were more likely to be disrupted than the formal-perceptual attributes in this study, suggesting that role of cultural factors in this assessment were not sufficient to obscure the effects of brain damage.
However, future studies that address both cultural and biological variables will be needed to provide a rich understanding of art apprehension. Another specific limit of this study is the sampling of brain regions. While 20 participants is a relatively large group of brain-damaged subjects, as mentioned above, we did not sample the ventral occipito-temporal or ventro-medial frontal cortices.
We have studies currently underway to probe these areas. Another limit is that we restricted ourselves to people with right brain damage. Since this is the first study of its kind, we did not wish to confound our results with concomitant language comprehension deficits that follow from left brain damage.
However, we have shown that art production can be profoundly affected by left brain damage (Smith et al., ). Given that production and perception must overlap at some representational levels, we would predict that left brain damage would also affect art perception. Again future studies will need to sort out the role of the left hemisphere in art perception. Finally, we recognize that the description and evaluation of art are qualitatively different. People are more likely to agree about whether or not an image has warm tones than to agree about whether or not the image is appealing.
This difference is evident even in our healthy participants in whom agreement on preference was the lowest than it was for any of the descriptive scales. Given that the evaluation of artwork is less stable than descriptive judgments, assessing the effects of brain damage in preference is also more difficult. The problem of separating variance inherent in individual differences from those produced by the effects of brain damage remains a methodological challenge. To summarize, we believe that neuropsychology will play an important role in advancing neuroesthetics. However, to date most neuropsychological reports related to art have been anecdotal and qualitative in nature.
For this field to mature as a science, we advocate the use of quantitative methods. Here, we offer one approach that uses both quantitative behavioral and lesion analyses examining the role of the right hemisphere in art perception and evaluation. Aharon I., Etcoff N., Ariely D., Chabris C., O’Connor E., Breiter H. Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron 32, 537–/S0896-621-3. Alajouanine T. Aphasia and artistic realization.
Brain 71, 229–/brain/71.3.229. Bates E., Wilson S., Saygin A., Dick F., Sereno M., Knight R., Dronkers N.
Voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. 6, 448–450. Bogousslavsky J., Boller F. Neurological Disorders in Famous Artists, Basel: Karger.
Brown S., Gao X., Tisdelle L., Eickhoff S. B., Liotti M. Naturalizing aesthetics: brain areas for aesthetic appraisal across sensory modalities. Neuroimage 58, 250–/j.neuroimage.2011.06.012. Cela-Conde C. J., Munar E., Maestu F., Nadal M., Capo M.
A., del Rio D., Lopez-Ibor J. S., Mirasso C., Marty G. Sex-related similarities and differences in the neural correlates of beauty. 106, 3847–3/pnas. Chatterjee A. Universal and relative aesthetics: a framework from cognitive neuroscience.
International Association of Empirical Aesthetics, Takarazuka, 289–292. Chatterjee A. A disorder of spatial attention,” in Neurological Foundations of Cognitive Neuroscience, ed. D’Esposito M., editor. (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; ), 1–26. Chatterjee A.
Prospects for a cognitive neuroscience of visual aesthetics. Arts 4, 55–59. Chatterjee A. The neuropsychology of visual artists. Neuropsychologia 42, 1568–1/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.011. Chatterjee A. A madness to the methods in cognitive neuroscience?
17, 847–/021085. Chatterjee A. The neuropsychology of visual art: conferring capacity. 74, 39–4910.1016/S0074-773-X. Chatterjee A. “Prospects for a neuropsychology of art,” in Neuroaesthetics, eds Skov M., Vartanian O., editors.
(Amityville, NY: Baywood Publishing Company; ), 131–143. Chatterjee A. Neuroaesthetics: a coming of age story. 23, 53–6210.1162/jocn.2010.21457. Chatterjee A., Thomas A., Smith S. E., Aguirre G.
The neural response to facial attractiveness. Neuropsychology 23, 135–/a0014430. Chatterjee A., Widick P., Sternschein R., Smith W. B., II, Bromberger B.
The assessment of art attributes. Arts 28, 207–/EM.28.2.f. Cupchik G. C., Gebotys R. The experience of time, pleasure, and interest during aesthetic episodes.
Arts 6, 1–1210.2190/D585-A68D-UNC2-NFDF. Fechner G. Vorschule der Aesthetik. Leipzig: Breitkopf & Hartel. Fellows L. K., Heberlein A. S., Morales D.
A., Shivde G., Waller S., Wu D. Method matters: an empirical study of impact in cognitive neuroscience.
17, 850–/021139. Heilman K. M., Watson R. T., Valenstein E. “Neglect and related disorders,” in Clinical Neuropsychology, eds Heilman K. M., Valenstein E., editors.
(New York: Oxford University Press; ), 279–336. Hekkert P., Van Wieringen P.
Beauty in the eye of expert and nonexpert beholders: a study in the appraisal of art. 109, 389–/1423013. Ishai A. Sex, beauty and the orbitofrontal cortex. 63, 181–/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.010.
Ishai A., Fairhall S., Pepperell R. Perception, memory and aesthetics of indeterminate art. 73, 319–/j.brainresbull.2007.04.009.
Jacobsen T., Schubotz R., Hofel L., Cramon D. Brain correlates of aesthetic judgments of beauty. Neuroimage 29, 276–/j.neuroimage.2005.07.010. Kable J. W., Glimcher P.
The neurobiology of decision: consensus and controversy. Neuron 63, 733–/j.neuron.2009.09.003. Kawabata H., Zeki S.
Neural correlates of beauty. 91, 1699–1/jn. Kimberg D. Y., Coslett H.
B., Schwartz M. Power in voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. 19, 1067–1/jocn.2007.19.7.1067. Leder H., Belke B., Oeberst A., Augustin D.
A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. 95, 489–/369811. Livingstone M. Vision and Art: The Biology of Seeing. New York: Abrams. Mattingley J., Berberovic N., Corben L., Slavin M., Nicholls M., Bradshaw J.
The greyscales task: a perceptual measure of attentional bias following unilateral hemispheric damage. Neuropsychologia 42, 387–/j.neuropsychologia.2003.07.007. Nadal M., Munar E., Capo M. A., Rosselo J., Cela-Conde C. Towards a framework for the study of the neural correlates of aesthetic preference. 21, 379–/84532653.
Nadal M., Pearce M. The Copenhagen neuroaesthetics conference: prospects and pitfalls for an emerging field. 76, 172–/j.bandc.2011.01.009. Ramachandran V. S., Hirstein H. The science of art: a neurological theory of aesthetic experience. 6, 15–51.
Rorden C., Karnath H.-O. Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age? 5, 813–/nrn1521. Russell P.
A., George D. Relationships between aesthetic response scales applied to paintings. Arts 8, 15–30. Skov M., Vartanian O., editors. Amityville, NY: Baywood Publishing Company, Inc. Smith W.
B., II, Sternschein R., Widick P., Bromberger B., Chatterjee A. Artistic production following brain damage. Leonardo 44, 405–/LEONa00240. van Buren B., Bromberger B., Potts D., Miller B., Chatterjee A.
“Changes in painting styles of artists with Alzheimer’s disease,” in International Association of Empirical Aesthetics Annual Meeting, Dresden. Vartanian O., Goel V. Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for paintings. Neuroreport 15, 893–/0002. Warrington E. The Visual Object and Spatial Perception Battery.
Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company. Winston J., O’Doherty J., Kilner J., Perrett D., Dolan R. Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia 45, 195–/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.009. Woods W. Parameters of aesthetic objects: applied aesthetics.
Arts 9, 105–114. Wu D. H., Waller S., Chatterjee A.
The functional neuroanatomy of thematic role and locative relational knowledge. 19, 1542–1/jocn.2007.19.9.1542. Zaidel D.
Neuropsychology of Art. New York: Psychology Press. Zeki S. Art and the brain.
If you are having trouble locating a specific resource, please visit the or the. The Online Writing Lab (OWL) at Purdue University houses writing resources and instructional material, and we provide these as a free service of the Writing Lab at Purdue. Students, members of the community, and users worldwide will find information to assist with many writing projects. Teachers and trainers may use this material for in-class and out-of-class instruction. For more information about services for the Purdue University community, including one-to-one consultations, ESL conversation groups and workshops, please visit the.
Mission The Purdue University Writing Lab and Purdue Online Writing Lab (OWL) assist clients in their development as writers—no matter what their skill level—with on-campus consultations, online participation, and community engagement. The Purdue Writing Lab serves the Purdue, West Lafayette, campus and coordinates with local literacy initiatives. The Purdue OWL offers global support through online reference materials and services.
Spotlight Resource Dec. 10, 2018: Visit our, which provides a quick crash course on the Chicago Manual of Style (CMOS) and directs visitors to medium-specific citation resources. Thanks to graduate student Maggie Myers for this much-needed update, which brings the navigation architecture of our Chicago resources in line with our MLA and APA resources. Need Help Navigating the New Site?. Social Media.